Human cooperation and social network expansion over time
It is generally held that human cooperation first evolved in our ancestral past, when we tended to live in small groups composed mostly of family members, and when cooperation could therefore be selected for by kin selection. This narrative is also sometimes invoked to explain the origin of cooperative mechanisms that are primarily about cooperation between nonkin. For example, in the iterated Prisoner’s Dilemma, cooperation between nonkin can be maintained by the tit-for-tat strategy, and a population of tit-for-tat players can resist invasion by defectors. However, tit-for-tat strategists cannot invade a population of defectors (presumably the primordial strategy), and so the question remains how it got started in the first place. Axelrod & Hamilton (1981) proposed that one way could have been if dispersal was low enough in the past that cooperative types tended to cluster together.
Over the course of the human lineage, there is a general pattern expanding social networks and declining relatedness between interacting individuals. DNA analysis back to 45 ka shows a general decline in background relatedness over time, with a marked change at the Neolithic Demographic Transition (Ringbauer et al., 2021), when the advent of farming in each region coincided with a sudden increase in population size (Bocquet-Appel, 2011). In industrialised societies, falling mortality and fertility has also reduced the size of kin networks (David-Barrett, 2019), motivating new bases for social identity (David-Barrett, 2020). In the popular imagination, the lifeways of hunter-gatherer people represent our closest analogue to what the deep ancestral past must have been like; however, modern hunter-gatherers also maintain expansive social networks with hundreds of unrelated individuals (Hill et al., 2011; Bird et al., 2019), and groups congregate seasonally for communal hunting and socialising (Kelly, 2013; Balme, 2018).
Not a lot is known about social structure before 45 ka (Graeber and Wengrow, 2018), and so inferences must be made on the basis of fossils and other material evidence. One potential indicator of social structure is the distance that materials were transported from their source. Before around 1.6 Ma, raw-material transport distances are comparable to chimpanzee home-range sizes (∼ 13 km), indicating relatively isolated social groups composed mostly of kin (Marwick, 2003). Distances and occurrences of material transport subsequently increased over the course of the Early and Middle Stone Age. For example, approximately 295-320 ka, obsidian and ochre were transported 25-50 km (as the crow flies) (Brooks et al., 2018). After ~130 ka, raw-material transport distances frequently exceeded 300 km (Marwick, 2003). These distances may be indicative of networks of exchange, which implies increased language abilities (Marwick, 2003) and notions of relatedness beyond genetic kin (Moutsiou, 2012). However, we must also be cautious when trying to infer what these exchanges meant, because humans are quirky and sometimes transport materials over long distances for unexpected reasons (Graeber and Wengrow, 2018).
The types of materials that are found can also be indicative of something social. Tool sophistication and symbolic development may be indicative of cognitive and social development, and their stylistic diversity indicative of cultures and the flow of information between them. In the early period (~1.5-0.4 Ma), evidence of material innovation is scarce; but nonetheless, encephalisation increased over this period, which some authors attribute to the demands of increasing social complexity (Gamble et al., 2011). The transport of ochre mentioned above, from 295-320 ka, is notable because ochre is used by modern people as a pigment, either for artwork or body ornamentation, and as a potential indicator of one’s group identity (Brooks et al., 2018).
The appearance of beads (> 142 ka Sehasseh et al., 2021) may be important because they can be used to communicate social identity (e.g., group membership and marital status) to strangers. The use of beads greatly increased around the same time that population sizes increased (40-45 ka, Kuhn et al., 2001), which further supports the idea that beads were used in this way. Beads were also transported long distances; for example, shell beads found in the Kimberly, Australia, from 30 ka, were transported > 300 km from their source (Balme and Morse, 2006). It is interesting to note that some modern hunter-gatherers use the exchange of beads as the substrate for indoctrinating children into socially defined notions of kinship (Wiessner, 1998). We also find long-distance transport of other materials potentially used to communicate identity, e.g., ochre from 32 ka transported 125 km in central Australia (Smith et al., 1998).
An expanding social network could have provided the opportunity to experiment with different styles of large-scale collective action (Graeber and Wengrow, 2021). For example, the use of nets is an indicator of communal hunting, particularly of the integration of labour from women, children, and the elderly (Soffer et al., 2001). Evidence of large-scale fishing operations occur from 27 ka in Australia (Balme, 1995).
Acquiring meat may have been one of our ancestors’ first collaborative activities, and collaborative foraging in general has been linked to the early stages of human cooperation (Tomasello et al. 2012). Unfortunately, it is unclear exactly how animal products were first acquired by our ancestors: were they working together to take down large prey in some way (Domínguez-Rodrigo et al., 2021), or were they opportunistically finding and/or snatching scraps from animals taken down by other carnivores (Pobiner, 2020)? Perhaps scavenging itself was a collaborative activity if that meant confronting and scaring away large, dangerous carnivores who were still at their meals (Bickerton & Szathmáry, 2011)?
In the figure below, I’ve sketched out a rough timeline of some of the elements above. This should help orient different narratives about how cooperation evolved.
References
Allington-Jones, L. (2015). The Clacton spear: The last one hundred years. Archaeological Journal, 172(2), 273-296.
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211(4489), 1390-1396.
Balme, J., Davidson, I., McDonald, J., Stern, N., & Veth, P. (2009). Symbolic behaviour and the peopling of the southern arc route to Australia. Quaternary International, 202(1-2), 59-68.
Balme, J. (1995). 30,000 years of fishery in western New South Wales, Archaeology in Oceania 30(1): 1–21.
Balme, J. (2018). Communal hunting by aboriginal australians: archaeological and ethnographic evidence, in K. Carlson and L. Bemet (eds), Manipulating Prey: Development of Large-Scale Kill Events Around the Globe, University of Colorado Press, Boulder, Colorado, pp. 42–62.
Balme, J. and Morse, K. (2006). Shell beads and social behaviour in Pleistocene Australia, antiquity 80(310): 799–811.
Bickerton, D., & Szathmáry, E. (2011). Confrontational scavenging as a possible source for language and cooperation. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 11(1), 1-7.
Bird, D. W., Bird, R. B., Codding, B. F. and Zeanah, D. W. (2019). Variability in the organization and size of hunter-gatherer groups: Foragers do not live in small-scale societies, Journal of human evolution 131: 96–108.
Bocquet-Appel, J.-P. (2011). When the world’s population took off: the springboard of the Neolithic Demographic Transition, Science 333(6042): 560–561.
Bramble, D. M., & Lieberman, D. E. (2004). Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. nature, 432(7015), 345-352.
Brooks, A. S., Yellen, J. E., Potts, R., Behrensmeyer, A. K., Deino, A. L., Leslie, D. E., Ambrose, S. H., Ferguson, J. R., d’Errico, F., Zipkin, A. M., Whittaker, S., Post, J., Veatch, E. G., Foecke, K. and Clark, J. B. (2018). Long-distance stone transport and pigment use in the earliest Middle Stone Age, Science 360(6384): 90–94.
Cunha, E. (2016). Compassion between humans since when? What the fossils tell us. Etnográfica. Revista do Centro em Rede de Investigação em Antropologia, 20(3)), 653-657.
David-Barrett, T. (2019). Network effects of demographic transition, Scientific Reports 9(1): 1–10.
David-Barrett, T. (2020). Herding friends in similarity-based architecture of social networks, Scientific Reports 10(1): 1–6.
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Pickering, T. R., Semaw, S., & Rogers, M. J. (2005). Cutmarked bones from Pliocene archaeological sites at Gona, Afar, Ethiopia: implications for the function of the world’s oldest stone tools. Journal of Human Evolution, 48(2), 109-121.
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Baquedano, E., Organista, E., Cobo-Sánchez, L., Mabulla, A., Maskara, V., Gidna, A., Pizarro-Monzo, M., Aramendi, J. et al. (2021) Early Pleistocene faunivorous hominins were not kleptoparasitic, and this impacted the evolution of human anatomy and socio-ecology. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1–13.
Gamble, C., Gowlett, J. and Dunbar, R. (2011). The social brain and the shape of the Palaeolithic, Cambridge Archaeological Journal 21(1): 115–136.
Graeber, D., & Wengrow, D. (2018). How to change the course of human history. Eurozine. Retrieved from https://www. eurozine. com/change-course-human-history.
Graeber, D. and Wengrow, D. (2021). The dawn of everything: A new history of humanity, Farrer, Straus and Giroux, New York.
Hill, K. R., Walker, R. S., Božičević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., Hurtado, A. M., Marlowe, F., Wiessner, P. and Wood, B. (2011). Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure, Science 331(6022): 1286–1289.
Kelly, R. L. (2013). The Lifeways of Hunter-Gatherers: The Foraging Spectrum, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Kuhn, S. L., Stiner, M. C., Reese, D. S. and Güleç, E. (2001). Ornaments of the earliest Upper Paleolithic: New insights from the Levant, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98(13): 7641–7646.
Lombard, M. (2016). Mountaineering or ratcheting? Stone Age hunting weapons as proxy for the evolution of human technological, behavioral and cognitive flexibility. In The nature of culture (pp. 135-146). Springer, Dordrecht.
Marwick, B. (2003). Pleistocene exchange networks as evidence for the evolution of language, Cambridge Archaeological Journal 13(1): 67–81.
Moutsiou, T. (2012). Changing scales of obsidian movement and social networking, Unravelling the Palaeolithic: Ten years of research at the Centre for the Archaeology of Human Origins (CAHO, University of Southampton), British Archaeological Reports, pp. 85–95.
Oakley, K. P., Andrews, P., Keeley, L. H., & Clark, J. D. (1977). A reappraisal of the Clacton spearpoint. In Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society (Vol. 43, pp. 13-30). Cambridge University Press.
Pobiner, B. L. (2020). The zooarchaeology and paleoecology of early hominin scavenging. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 29(2), 68-82.
Ringbauer, H., Novembre, J. and Steinrücken, M. (2021). Parental relatedness through time revealed by runs of homozygosity in ancient dna, Nature Communications 12(1): 1–11.
Soffer, O., Adovasio, J. M., & Hyland, D. C. (2001). Perishable technologies and invisible people: nets, baskets, and “Venus” wear ca. 26,000 BP. Enduring Records: the Environmental and Cultural Heritage, 233-45.
Thompson, J. C., Carvalho, S., Marean, C. W., & Alemseged, Z. (2019). Origins of the human predatory pattern: The transition to large-animal exploitation by early hominins. Current Anthropology, 60(1), 1-23.
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E. & Herrmann, E. (2012) Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673–692.
Sehasseh, E. M., Fernandez, P., Kuhn, S., Stiner, M., Mentzer, S., Colarossi, D., Clark, A., Lanoe, F., Pailes, M., Hoffmann, D. et al. (2021). Early Middle Stone Age personal ornaments from Bizmoune Cave, Essaouira, Morocco, Science Advances 7(39): eabi8620.
Smith, M., Fankhauser, B. and Jercher, M. (1998). The changing provenance of red ochre at puritjarra rock shelter, central australia: Late pleistocene to present, 64: 275–292.
Wiessner, P. (1998). Indoctrinability and the evolution of socially defined kinship, in I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt and F. Salter (eds), Indoctrinability, ideology and warfare: evolutionary perspectives, Berghahn Books, Oxford, pp. 133–150.
Wilkins, J., Schoville, B. J., Brown, K. S., & Chazan, M. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science, 338(6109), 942-946.